In color science, the dominant wavelength
(and the corresponding complementary wavelength) are ways of characterizing any
light mixture in terms of the monochromatic spectral light that evokes an
identical (and the corresponding opposite) perception of hue. For a given
physical light mixture, the dominant and complementary wavelengths are not
entirely fixed, but vary according to the illuminating light's precise color,
called the white point, due to the color constancy of vision.
Definitions
On the CIE color coordinate space , a
straight line drawn between the point for a given color and the point for the
color of the illuminant can be extrapolated out so that it intersects the
perimeter of the space in two points. The point of intersection nearer to the
color in question reveals the dominant wavelength of the color as the
wavelength of the pure spectral color at that intersection point. The point of
intersection on the opposite side of the color space gives the complementary
wavelength, which when added to the color in question in the right proportion
will yield the color of the illuminant (since the illuminant point necessarily
sits between these points on a straight line in CIE space, according to the
definition just given).
In situations where no particular
illuminant is specified, it is common to discuss dominant wavelength relative
to one of several "white" standard illuminants, such as equal-energy
(flat spectrum) or a color temperature such as 6500K. For the purposes of this
geometrical discussion, an analogy may be observed between the horseshoe shaped
CIE 1931 color space and a circular slice of HSV color space, where the CIE
flat spectrum white point at (1/3,1/3) is analogous to the HSV white point at
(0,0). This comparison clarifies the derivation of the ideas of hue and complementary
color common in uses of the HSV space.
Explanation
The psychological perception of color is
commonly thought of as a function of the power spectrum of light frequencies
impinging on the photoreceptors of the retina. In the simplest case of pure
spectral light (also known as monochromatic), the spectrum of the light has
power only in one narrow frequency band peak. For these simple stimuli, there
exists a continuum of perceived colors which changes as the frequency of the
narrow band peak is changed. This is the well known rainbow spectrum, which
ranges from red at one end to blue and violet at the other (corresponding
respectively to the long-wavelength and short-wavelength extremes of the
visible range of electromagnetic radiation).
However, light in the natural world is
almost never purely monochromatic; most natural light sources and reflected
light from natural objects comprise spectra that have complex profiles, with
varying power over many different frequencies. A naive perspective might be
that therefore all these different complex spectra would generate color
perceptions completely different from those evoked in the rainbow of pure
spectral light. One can perhaps see intuitively that this is not correct:
almost all hues in the natural world (purples being the exception, see below)
are represented in the pure rainbow spectrum, although they may be darker or
less saturated than they appear in the rainbow. How is it that all the complex
spectra in the natural world can be condensed to hues in the rainbow, which
only represents simple monochromatic band peak spectra? This is the result of
the design of the eye: the three color photoreceptors in the retina (the cones)
reduce the information in the light spectrum down to three activity coordinates.
Thus, many different physical light spectra converge psychologically to the
same perceived color. In effect, for any single color perception, there is a
whole parametric space in the power/frequency domain that maps to that one
color.
For many power distributions of natural
light, the set of spectra mapping to the same color perception also includes a
stimulus that is a narrow band at a single frequency; i.e. a pure spectral
light (usually with some flat spectrum white light added to desaturate). The
wavelength of this pure spectral light that will evoke the same color
perception as the given complicated light mixture is the dominant wavelength of
that mixture.
Note that since purples (mixtures of red
and blue/violet) cannot be pure spectral colors, no color mixture perceived as
purple in hue can be assigned a proper dominant wavelength. However, purple
mixtures can be assigned a proper complementary wavelength in the greenish
range, on the opposite side of the white point, and a "dominant hue"
as a non-spectral coordinate along the line of purples. See CIE for the
standard representation of color space, where the border is composed of a
horseshoe curve representing the pure spectral colors, with a straight line
completing the perimeter along the bottom and representing the mixtures of
extreme red and blue/violet that give the pure purples. The same argument
applies to complementary colors; for many coordinates in the green area of CIE
color space, there is a proper dominant wavelength but no proper complementary
wavelength, but there is a complementary purple hue.
Source From Wikipedia
没有评论:
发表评论