Dichromacy is the state of having two types of
functioning color receptors, called cone cells, in the eyes. Organisms with
dichromacy are called dichromats. Dichromats can match any color they see with
a mixture of no more than two pure spectral lights. By comparison, trichromats
require three pure spectral lights to match all colors that they can perceive,
and tetrachromats require four.
Dichromacy in humans is a color vision
defect in which one of the three basic color mechanisms is absent or not
functioning. It is hereditary and sex-linked, predominantly affecting males.
Dichromacy occurs when one of the cone pigments is missing and color is reduced
to two dimensions.
Classification
There are various kinds of color blindness:
Protanopia is a severe form of red-green
color blindness, in which there is impairment in perception of very long
wavelengths, such as reds. To these individuals, reds are perceived as beige or
grey and greens tend to look beige or grey like reds. It is also the most
common type of dichromacy today. This problem occurs because patients do not
have the red cone cells in the retina. Protanomaly is a less severe version.
Deuteranopia consists of an impairment in
perceiving medium wavelengths, such as greens. Deuteranomaly is a less severe
form of deuteranopia. Those with deuteranomaly cannot see reds and greens like
those without this condition; however, they can still distinguish them in most
cases. It is very similar to protanopia. In this form, patients do not have
green cone cells in the retina, which makes it hard to see the green color.
A rarer form of color blindness is
tritanopia, where there exists an inability to perceive short wavelengths, such
as blues. Sufferers have trouble distinguishing between yellow and blue. They
tend to confuse greens and blues, and yellow can appear pink. This is the rarest
of all dichromacy, and occurs in around 1 in 100,000 people. Patients do not
have the blue cone cells in the retina.
Testing for dichromacy
The three determining elements of a
dichromatic opponent-colour space are the missing colour, the null-luminance
plane, and the null-chrominance plane. The description of the phenomena itself
does not indicate the colour that is impaired to the dichromat, however, it
does provides enough information to identify the fundamental colour space, the
colours that are seen by the dichromat. This is based on testing both the
null-chrominance plane and null-luminance plane which intersect on the missing
colour. The cones excited to a corresponding colour in the colour space are
visible to the dichromat and those that are not excited are the missing
colours.
Animals that are dichromats
It is more informative to use situations
where less than the total visual system is operating when studying about
vision. For example, a system by which cones are the sole visual receptors
could be used. This is rare in humans but certain animals possess this trait
and this proves useful in understanding the concept of dichromacy.
While their Triassic ancestors were
trichromatic, placental mammals are as a rule dichromatic; the ability to see
long wavelengths (and thus separate green and red) was lost in the ancestor of
placental mammals, though it has been retained in some marsupials, where four
cases of trichromatic vision have been found. Recent genetic and behavioral
evidence suggests the South American marsupial Didelphis albiventris is
dichromatic, with only two classes of cone opsins having been found within the genus
Didelphis. Dichromatic vision may improve an animal's ability to distinguish
colours in dim light; the typically nocturnal nature of mammals, therefore, may
have led to the evolution of dichromacy as the basal mode of vision in
placental animals.
The exceptions to dichromatic vision in
placental mammals are primates closely related to humans, which are usually
trichromats, and sea mammals (both pinnipeds and cetaceans) which are cone
monochromats. New World Monkeys are a partial exception: in most species, males
are dichromats, and about 60% of females are trichromats, but the owl monkeys
are cone monochromats, and both sexes of howler monkeys are trichromats.
Color detecting abilities of dichromats
According to colour vision researchers at
the Medical College of Wisconsin (including Jay Neitz), each of the three
standard colour-detecting cones in the retina of trichromats – blue, green and
red – can pick up about 100 different gradations of colour. If each detector is
independent of the others, simple exponentiation gives a total number of
colours discernible by an average human as their product, or about 1 million;
nevertheless, other researchers have put the number at upwards of 2.3 million.
Exponentiation suggests that a dichromat (such as a human with red-green color
blindness) would be able to distinguish about 10,000 different colours, but no
such calculation has been verified by psychophysical testing.
Furthermore, dichromats have a
significantly higher threshold than trichromats for coloured stimuli flickering
at low (1 Hz) frequencies. At higher (10 or 16 Hz) frequencies, dichromats
perform as well as or better than trichromats.. This means such animals would
still observe the flicker instead of a temporally fused visual percept as is
the case in human movie watching at a high enough frame rate.
Source From Wikipedia
没有评论:
发表评论